Visual acuity (English Wikipedia)

Analysis of information sources in references of the Wikipedia article "Visual acuity" in English language version.

refsWebsite
Global rank English rank
4th place
4th place
2nd place
2nd place
11th place
8th place
5th place
5th place
1st place
1st place
18th place
17th place
low place
low place
120th place
125th place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
6th place
6th place
441st place
311th place
2,888th place
2,730th place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
low place
459th place
360th place
6,658th place
4,027th place
332nd place
246th place

archive.org

cornell.edu

law.cornell.edu

doi.org

harvard.edu

ui.adsabs.harvard.edu

hathitrust.org

babel.hathitrust.org

icoph.org

journalofvision.org

lea-test.fi

legislation.gov.au

michaelbach.de

ndt-ed.org

nih.gov

pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

optometricmanagement.com

researchgate.net

semanticscholar.org

api.semanticscholar.org

  • Grimm; Rassow; Wesemann; Saur; Hilz (1994). "Correlation of optotypes with the Landolt Ring – a fresh look at the comparability of optotypes". Optometry and Vision Science. 71 (1): 6–13. doi:10.1097/00006324-199401000-00002. PMID 8146001. S2CID 24533843.
  • Original figure in Østerberg, G. (1935). "Topography of the layer of rods and cones in the human retina". Acta Ophthalmologica. 13 (Suppl. 6): 11–103. doi:10.1111/j.1755-3768.1935.tb04723.x. S2CID 220560741.. Østerberg's figure is reproduced in Strasburger et al. (2011), Fig. 4
  • Weinberger H (19 February 1971). "Conjecture on the Visual Estimation of Relative Radial Motion". Nature. 229 (5286): 562. Bibcode:1971Natur.229..562W. doi:10.1038/229562a0. PMID 4925353. S2CID 4290244.
  • Schrater PR, Knill DC, Simoncelli EP (12 April 2001). "Perceiving visual expansion without optic flow". Nature. 410 (6830): 816–819. Bibcode:2001Natur.410..816S. doi:10.1038/35071075. PMID 11298449. S2CID 4406675. When an observer moves forward in the environment, the image on his or her retina expands. The rate of this expansion conveys information about the observer's speed and the time to collision... this rate might also be estimated from changes in the size (or scale) of image features... we show, ... observers can estimate expansion rates from scale-change information alone, and that pure scale changes can produce motion after-effects. These two findings suggest that the visual system contains mechanisms that are explicitly sensitive to changes in scale.
  • Maddox ME, Kiefer A (September 2012). "Looming Threshold Limits and Their Use in Forensic Practice". Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting. 50 (1): 700–704. doi:10.1177/1071181312561146. S2CID 109898296. A number of laboratory researchers have reported values of the looming threshold to be in the range of 0.003 radian/sec. Forensic practitioners routinely use elevated values of the looming threshold, e.g., 0.005–0.008, to account for the complexity of real-world driving tasks. However, only one source has used data from actual vehicle accidents to arrive at a looming threshold – and that value, 0.0275 rad/sec, is an order of magnitude larger than that derived from laboratory studies. In this study, we examine a much broader range of real-world accident data to obtain an estimate of the reasonable upper end of the looming threshold. The results show a range of 0.0397 to 0.0117 rad/sec...

ski.org

swift.ac.uk

umich.edu

animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu

  • Kirschbaum K. "Family Accipitridae". Animal Diversity Web. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology. Retrieved 30 January 2010.

visionscience.com

web.archive.org

whonamedit.com

worldcat.org

search.worldcat.org