Richard Lynn (Spanish Wikipedia)

Analysis of information sources in references of the Wikipedia article "Richard Lynn" in Spanish language version.

refsWebsite
Global rank Spanish rank
2nd place
2nd place
1st place
1st place
6th place
5th place
3rd place
7th place
9,263rd place
low place
57th place
3rd place
1,785th place
2,882nd place
20th place
26th place
4th place
4th place
70th place
166th place
30th place
65th place
8th place
18th place
low place
low place
219th place
102nd place
2,003rd place
4,800th place
1,880th place
1,636th place
595th place
6,630th place
11th place
79th place
12th place
30th place

archive.org

bbc.co.uk

bbc.com

books.google.com

dailymail.co.uk

doi.org

dx.doi.org

  • Hunt, E; Wittmann, W. (2008). «National intelligence and national prosperity». Intelligence 36 (1): 1-9. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002. 
  • Valone, David A. (2002). «Richard Lynn: Eugenics: A Reassessment, review». Isis 93 (3): 534. doi:10.1086/374143. 
  • Barnett, Susan M.; Williams, Wendy (2004). «National Intelligence and The Emperor's New Clothes». PsycCRITIQUES 49 (4): 389-396. doi:10.1037/004367. «Among this book's strengths are that it argues for a point of view unpopular within the scientific community, it relies on hard data to make its points, its organisation and clarity. Also, the book is expansive in its thinking and argumentation. All of these strengths considered, however, we believe that the arguments advanced in the book are flawed by an omnipresent logical fallacy and confusion of correlation with causation that undermines the foundation of the book.» 
  • Neisser, Ulric (2004). «Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists?». PsycCRITIQUES 49 (1): 5-7. doi:10.1037/004224. 
  • Gelb, Steven A. (1997), «Heart of Darkness: The Discreet Charm of the Hereditarian Psychologist», Review of Education, Pedagogy, and Cultural Studies 19 (1): 129-139, doi:10.1080/1071441970190110 .
  • Kenny, Michael G. (2002). «Toward a racial abyss: eugenics, Wickliffe Draper, and the origins of The Pioneer Fund». Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 38 (3): 259-283. ISSN 0022-5061. PMID 12115787. doi:10.1002/jhbs.10063. Consultado el 13 de abril de 2021. 
  • Mehler, Barry (1989). «Foundation for fascism: The new eugenics movement in the United States». Patterns of Prejudice 23 (4): 17-25. doi:10.1080/0031322x.1989.9970026. 
  • Newby, Robert G.; Newby, Diane E. (1995). «The Bell Curve: Another Chapter in the Continuing Political Economy of Racism». American Behavioral Scientist 39 (1): 12-24. doi:10.1177/0002764295039001003. 
  • Lynn, Richard; Irwing, Paul (September 2004). «Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis». Intelligence 32 (5): 481-498. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.008. 
  • Nyborg, Helmuth (July 2012). «A conversation with Richard Lynn». Personality and Individual Differences 53 (2): 79-84. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.033. 
  • Rojahn, Johannes; Naglieri, Jack A. (1 de mayo de 2006). «Developmental gender differences on the Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test in a nationally normed sample of 5–17 year olds». Intelligence 34 (3): 253-260. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.004. 
  • Hamilton, W. D. (2000). «A review of Dysgenics: Genetic Deterioration in Modern Populations». Ann. Hum. Genet. 64 (4): 363-374. doi:10.1046/j.1469-1809.2000.6440363.x. 
  • Jackson, John P. (June 2006). «Argumentum Ad Hominem in the Science of Race». Argumentation and Advocacy (en inglés) 43 (1): 14-28. ISSN 1051-1431. S2CID 142449810. doi:10.1080/00028533.2006.11821659. 
  • Wicherts, J. M.; Dolan, C. V.; van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010). «A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans». Intelligence 38 (1): 1-20. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002. 
  • Skeem, Jennifer L. (2003). «Psychopathic personality and racial/ethnic differences reconsidered: a reply to Lynn (2002)». Personality and Individual Differences 35 (6): 1439-1462. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00361-6. 

du.edu

mysite.du.edu

  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «Lynn's distortions and misrepresentations of the data constitute a truly venomous racism, combined with scandalous disregard for scientific objectivity. Lynn is widely known among academics to be an associate editor of the racist journal "Mankind Quarterly" and a major recipient of financial support from the nativist, eugenically oriented Pioneer Fund.» 
  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American 272. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «In 1992 Owen reported on a sample of coloured students that had been added to the groups he had tested earlier. The footnote in "The Bell Curve" seems to credit this report as proving that South African colored students have an IQ "similar to that of American blacks", that is, about 85 (the actual reference does not appear in the book's bibliography). That statement does not correctly characterize Owen's work. The test used by Owen in 1992 was the "nonverbal" Raven's Progressive Matrices, which is thought to be less culturally biased than other IQ tests. He was able to compare the performance of colored students with that of the whites, blacks and Indians in his 1989 study because the earlier set of pupils had taken the Progressive Matrices in addition to the Junior Aptitude Tests. The black pupils, recall, had poor knowledge of English, but Owen felt that the instructions for the Matrices "are so easy that they can be explained with gestures". Owen's 1992 paper again does not assign IQs to the pupils. Rather he gives the mean number of correct responses on the Progressive Matrices (out of a possible 60) for each group: 45 for whites, 42 for Indians, 37 for coloreds and 28 for blacks. The test's developer, John Raven, repeatedly insisted that results on the Progressive Matrices tests cannot be converted into IQs. Matrices scores, unlike IQs, are not symmetrical around their mean (no "bell curve" here). There is thus no meaningful way to convert an average of raw Matrices scores into an IQ, and no comparison with American black IQs is possible.» 
  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American 272. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «Lynn chose to ignore the substance of Crawford-Nutt's paper, which reported that 228 black high school students in Soweto scored an average of 45 correct responses on the Matrices—HIGHER than the mean of 44 achieved by the same-age white sample on whom the test's norms had been established and well above the mean of Owen's coloured pupils.» 

duke.edu

biology.duke.edu

  • Kamin, Leon. «Behind the Bell Curve». Scientific American: 100. Archivado desde el original el 5 de marzo de 2016. «Lynn's distortions and misrepresentations of the data constitute a truly venomous racism, combined with the scandalous disregard for scientific objectivity». 

issn.org

portal.issn.org

loc.gov

id.loc.gov

mankindquarterly.org

nih.gov

pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

nybooks.com

semanticscholar.org

api.semanticscholar.org

splcenter.org

  • «Richard Lynn». Southern Poverty Law Center. Consultado el 7 de febrero de 2016. 

telegraph.co.uk

theguardian.com

tribune.com.pk

blogs.tribune.com.pk

web.archive.org

  • Kamin, Leon. «Behind the Bell Curve». Scientific American: 100. Archivado desde el original el 5 de marzo de 2016. «Lynn's distortions and misrepresentations of the data constitute a truly venomous racism, combined with the scandalous disregard for scientific objectivity». 
  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «Lynn's distortions and misrepresentations of the data constitute a truly venomous racism, combined with scandalous disregard for scientific objectivity. Lynn is widely known among academics to be an associate editor of the racist journal "Mankind Quarterly" and a major recipient of financial support from the nativist, eugenically oriented Pioneer Fund.» 
  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American 272. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «In 1992 Owen reported on a sample of coloured students that had been added to the groups he had tested earlier. The footnote in "The Bell Curve" seems to credit this report as proving that South African colored students have an IQ "similar to that of American blacks", that is, about 85 (the actual reference does not appear in the book's bibliography). That statement does not correctly characterize Owen's work. The test used by Owen in 1992 was the "nonverbal" Raven's Progressive Matrices, which is thought to be less culturally biased than other IQ tests. He was able to compare the performance of colored students with that of the whites, blacks and Indians in his 1989 study because the earlier set of pupils had taken the Progressive Matrices in addition to the Junior Aptitude Tests. The black pupils, recall, had poor knowledge of English, but Owen felt that the instructions for the Matrices "are so easy that they can be explained with gestures". Owen's 1992 paper again does not assign IQs to the pupils. Rather he gives the mean number of correct responses on the Progressive Matrices (out of a possible 60) for each group: 45 for whites, 42 for Indians, 37 for coloreds and 28 for blacks. The test's developer, John Raven, repeatedly insisted that results on the Progressive Matrices tests cannot be converted into IQs. Matrices scores, unlike IQs, are not symmetrical around their mean (no "bell curve" here). There is thus no meaningful way to convert an average of raw Matrices scores into an IQ, and no comparison with American black IQs is possible.» 
  • Kamin, Leon (February 1995). «The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life». Scientific American 272. Archivado desde el original el 22 de octubre de 2007. «Lynn chose to ignore the substance of Crawford-Nutt's paper, which reported that 228 black high school students in Soweto scored an average of 45 correct responses on the Matrices—HIGHER than the mean of 44 achieved by the same-age white sample on whom the test's norms had been established and well above the mean of Owen's coloured pupils.»