Petty, Richard; Cacioppo, John (1986). «The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion». Advances in Experimental Social Psychology19. pp. 123-181. ISBN9780120152193. doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60214-2.
Devine, Patricia (1989). «Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology58: 5-18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5.
Alos-Ferrer, C.; Strack, F. (2014). «From dual processes to multiple selves: Implications for economic behavior». Journal of Economic Psychology41: 1-11. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2013.12.005.
Baron, Jonathan; Scott, Sydney; Fincher, Katrina; Metz, Emlen (2015). «Why does the Cognitive Reflection Test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)?». Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition4 (3): 265-284. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.09.003.
Bago, Bence; De Neys, Wim (2018). «The intuitive greater good: Testing the corrective dual process model of moral cognition». Journal of Experimental Psychology: General148 (10): 1782-1801. PMID30550336. doi:10.1037/xge0000533.
Pennycook, Gordon; Cheyne, James Allan; Koehler, Derek. J.; Fugelsang, Jonathan A. (2015). «Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition?». Behavior Research Methods48 (1): 341-348. PMID25740762. doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0576-1.
Evans, J. (2012). «Questions and challenges for the new psychology of reasoning». Thinking & Reasoning18 (1): 5-31. doi:10.1080/13546783.2011.637674.
Weidenfeld, A.; Oberauer, K.; Hornig, R. (2005). «Causal and non causal conditionals: an integrated model of interpretation and reasoning». The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 58A (8): 1479-1513. PMID16365951. doi:10.1080/02724980443000719.
Sun, R.; Zhang, X.; Mathews, R. (2009). «Capturing human data in a letter counting task: Accessibility and action-centeredness in representing cognitive skills». Neural Networks22 (1): 15-29. PMID18804953. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2008.08.004.
Reyna, V.F.; Estrada, S.M.; DeMarinis, J.A.; Myers, R.M.; Stanisz, J.M.; Mills, B.A. (2011). «Neurobiological and memory models of risky decision making in adolescents versus young adults». Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition37 (5): 1125-1142. PMID21707215. doi:10.1037/a0023943.
Bago, Bence; De Neys, Wim (2018). «The intuitive greater good: Testing the corrective dual process model of moral cognition». Journal of Experimental Psychology: General148 (10): 1782-1801. PMID30550336. doi:10.1037/xge0000533.
Pennycook, Gordon; Cheyne, James Allan; Koehler, Derek. J.; Fugelsang, Jonathan A. (2015). «Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition?». Behavior Research Methods48 (1): 341-348. PMID25740762. doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0576-1.
Weidenfeld, A.; Oberauer, K.; Hornig, R. (2005). «Causal and non causal conditionals: an integrated model of interpretation and reasoning». The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 58A (8): 1479-1513. PMID16365951. doi:10.1080/02724980443000719.
Sun, R.; Zhang, X.; Mathews, R. (2009). «Capturing human data in a letter counting task: Accessibility and action-centeredness in representing cognitive skills». Neural Networks22 (1): 15-29. PMID18804953. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2008.08.004.
Reyna, V.F.; Estrada, S.M.; DeMarinis, J.A.; Myers, R.M.; Stanisz, J.M.; Mills, B.A. (2011). «Neurobiological and memory models of risky decision making in adolescents versus young adults». Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition37 (5): 1125-1142. PMID21707215. doi:10.1037/a0023943.