Konspirasjonsteori (Norwegian Wikipedia)

Analysis of information sources in references of the Wikipedia article "Konspirasjonsteori" in Norwegian language version.

refsWebsite
Global rank Norwegian rank
2nd place
9th place
5th place
13th place
1st place
1st place
222nd place
262nd place
849th place
2nd place
low place
61st place
1,595th place
3rd place
149th place
236th place
581st place
1,575th place
12th place
35th place
low place
2,106th place
low place
low place
low place
167th place
731st place
686th place
low place
low place
40th place
17th place
low place
low place
low place
658th place
low place
63rd place
1,677th place
6th place
low place
171st place
low place
818th place
3,142nd place
4,691st place
234th place
369th place
6,703rd place
low place
low place
low place
488th place
1,815th place
low place
1,852nd place
low place
low place
8th place
21st place
low place
low place
5,913th place
8,283rd place

1911encyclopedia.org

54.1911encyclopedia.org

abcnyheter.no

bbc.co.uk

news.bbc.co.uk

bps.org.uk

britannica.com

cjc-online.ca

columbia.edu

dagbladet.no

doi.org

  • Napolitano, M. Giulia; Reuter, Kevin (24. september 2021). «What is a Conspiracy Theory?». Erkenntnis (på engelsk). ISSN 1572-8420. doi:10.1007/s10670-021-00441-6. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Stojanov, Ana; Halberstadt, Jamin (1. juli 2019). «The Conspiracy Mentality Scale». Social Psychology. 4. 50: 215–232. ISSN 1864-9335. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000381. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Kjetil Braut Simonsen (desember 2017). «Vidkun Quisling, antisemittismen og den paranoide stil | HT». Historisk tidsskrift (på norsk). doi:10.18261/issn.1504-2944-2017-03-04. Besøkt 8. mai 2022. 
  • Coady, David (27. april 2021). «Conspiracy theory as heresy». Educational Philosophy and Theory. 0. 0: 1–5. ISSN 0013-1857. doi:10.1080/00131857.2021.1917364. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Clarke, Steve (2002). «Conspiracy Theories and Conspiracy Theorizing». Philosophy of the Social Sciences. 2 (på engelsk). 32: 131–150. ISSN 0048-3931. doi:10.1177/004931032002001. Besøkt 6. november 2019. 
  • Bangstad, S. (2017). Rasebegrepets fortid og nåtid. Norsk sosiologisk tidsskrift 1 (03): 233–51. https://doi.org/10.18261/issn.2535-2512-2017-03-03
  • Sunstein, Cass R.; Vermeule, Adrian (2009). «Conspiracy Theories: Causes and Cures». Journal of Political Philosophy. 2 (på engelsk). 17: 202–227. ISSN 1467-9760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «“THE truth is out there”: conspiracy theories are all around us. In August 2004, a poll by Zogby International showed that 49 percent of New York City residents, with a margin of error of 3.5 percent, believed that officials of the U.S. government “knew in advance that attacks were planned on or around September 11, 2001, and that they consciously failed to act.” In a Scripps‐Howard Poll in 2006, some 36 percent of respondents assented to the claim that “federal officials either participated in the attacks on the World Trade Center or took no action to stop them.” Sixteen percent said that it was either very likely or somewhat likely that “the collapse of the twin towers in New York was aided by explosives secretly planted in the two buildings.”» 
  • Barkun, Michael (1. mars 1996). «Religion, militias and Oklahoma city: The mind of conspiratorialists». Terrorism and Political Violence. 1. 8: 50–64. ISSN 0954-6553. doi:10.1080/09546559608427332. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Crenshaw, Martha (2000). «The Psychology of Terrorism: An Agenda for the 21st Century». Political Psychology. 2 (på engelsk). 21: 405–420. ISSN 0162-895X. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00195. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Barkun, Michael (4. mai 2017). «President Trump and the “Fringe”». Terrorism and Political Violence. 3. 29: 437–443. ISSN 0954-6553. doi:10.1080/09546553.2017.1313649. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Kjetil Braut Simonsen. Butter, Michael; Knight, Peter, red. «Antisemitism and Conspiracism (kapittel 3.8)». Routledge Handbook of Conspiracy Theories. doi:10.4324/9780429452734. 
  • Swami, Viren; Voracek, Martin; Stieger, Stefan; Tran, Ulrich S.; Furnham, Adrian (1. desember 2014). «Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories». Cognition. 3. 133: 572–585. ISSN 0010-0277. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Belief in conspiracy theories has been associated with a range of negative health, civic, and social outcomes, requiring reliable methods of reducing such belief. Thinking dispositions have been highlighted as one possible factor associated with belief in conspiracy theories, but actual relationships have only been infrequently studied. In Study 1, we examined associations between belief in conspiracy theories and a range of measures of thinking dispositions in a British sample (N = 990). Results indicated that a stronger belief in conspiracy theories was significantly associated with lower analytic thinking and open-mindedness and greater intuitive thinking. In Studies 2–4, we examined the causational role played by analytic thinking in relation to conspiracist ideation. In Study 2 (N = 112), we showed that a verbal fluency task that elicited analytic thinking reduced belief in conspiracy theories. In Study 3 (N = 189), we found that an alternative method of eliciting analytic thinking, which related to cognitive disfluency, was effective at reducing conspiracist ideation in a student sample. In Study 4, we replicated the results of Study 3 among a general population sample (N = 140) in relation to generic conspiracist ideation and belief in conspiracy theories about the July 7, 2005, bombings in London. Our results highlight the potential utility of supporting attempts to promote analytic thinking as a means of countering the widespread acceptance of conspiracy theories.» 
  • Wood, Michael J.; Douglas, Karen M.; Sutton, Robbie M. (25. januar 2012). «Dead and Alive». Social Psychological and Personality Science. 6 (på engelsk). 3: 767–773. ISSN 1948-5506. doi:10.1177/1948550611434786. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: A self-sustaining worldview comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated in endorsement. In Study 1 (n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another but by broader beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.» 
  • Oliver, J. Eric; Wood, Thomas J. (2014). «Conspiracy Theories and the Paranoid Style(s) of Mass Opinion». American Journal of Political Science. 4 (på engelsk). 58: 952–966. ISSN 1540-5907. doi:10.1111/ajps.12084. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Although conspiracy theories have long been a staple of American political culture, no research has systematically examined the nature of their support in the mass public. Using four nationally representative surveys, sampled between 2006 and 2011, we find that half of the American public consistently endorses at least one conspiracy theory and that many popular conspiracy theories are differentiated along ideological and anomic dimensions. In contrast with many theoretical speculations, we do not find conspiracism to be a product of greater authoritarianism, ignorance, or political conservatism. Rather, the likelihood of supporting conspiracy theories is strongly predicted by a willingness to believe in other unseen, intentional forces and an attraction to Manichean narratives. These findings both demonstrate the widespread allure of conspiracy theories as political explanations and offer new perspectives on the forces that shape mass opinion and American political culture. … Self‐described liberals score over one point lower, on average, on the ideological conspiracy scale than self‐described conservatives.» 
  • Imhoff, Roland; Zimmer, Felix; Klein, Olivier; António, João H. C.; Babinska, Maria; Bangerter, Adrian; Bilewicz, Michal; Blanuša, Nebojša; Bovan, Kosta (17. januar 2022). «Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries». Nature Human Behaviour (på engelsk): 1–12. ISSN 2397-3374. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7. Besøkt 30. januar 2022. «We conclude that conspiracy mentality is associated with extreme left- and especially extreme right-wing beliefs, and that this non-linear relation may be strengthened by, but is not reducible to, deprivation of political control.» 
  • Uscinski, Joseph E. (2014). «Conspiracy Theories Are for Losers». American Conspiracy Theories. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-935180-0. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199351800.001.0001/acprof-9780199351800-chapter-6. Besøkt 30. januar 2022. 
  • Eberl, Jakob-Moritz; Huber, Robert A.; Greussing, Esther (31. mai 2021). «From populism to the “plandemic”: why populists believe in COVID-19 conspiracies». Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. sup1. 31: 272–284. ISSN 1745-7289. doi:10.1080/17457289.2021.1924730. Besøkt 17. desember 2021. «[T]he complex nature of the COVID-19 pandemic makes it an ideal playground for populists’ opposition to scientific and political elites. […] We demonstrate a negative correlation of populist attitudes with both trust in political and scientific institutions, which, in return, negatively relate to COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. This results in an overall positive relationship of populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs that is independent of political ideology.» 
  • Hornsey, Matthew J.; Finlayson, Matthew; Chatwood, Gabrielle; Begeny, Christopher T. (1. mai 2020). «Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (på engelsk). 88: 103947. ISSN 0022-1031. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103947. Besøkt 17. desember 2021. 
  • Jolley, Daniel; Douglas, Karen M. (februar 2014). «The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one's carbon footprint». British Journal of Psychology (på engelsk). 105 (1): 35–56. doi:10.1111/bjop.12018. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 

faktisk.no

tenk.faktisk.no

forskning.no

  • Forskning.no om konspirasjonsteorier
  • Dyrendal, Asbjørn; NTNU (18. mai 2006). «Kronikk: Sammensvergelser på bestselgerlistene». forskning.no (på norsk). Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Den pensjonerte journalisten Per-Aslak Ertresvåg har oppdaget amerikansk konspirasjonstenkning og svelger teoriene med søkke og snøre. Resultatet selges i tusentall, skriver religionsviteren Asbjørn Dyrendal i denne kronikken om boka "Makten bak makten".» 
  • Ingrid Spilde: «Hvem tror på konspirasjonsteorier?» Arkivert 2014-02-20, hos Wayback Machine., forskning.no, 24. april 2012: «Mennesker som er tilhengere av én konspirasjonsteori har en større tendens til å også tro på andre teorier, selv om det ikke er noen logisk eller tematisk sammenheng mellom dem.»

fritanke.no

globaloutlook.ca

hogrefe.com

econtent.hogrefe.com

hoover.org

idunn.no

michaelshermer.com

nature.com

  • Imhoff, Roland; Zimmer, Felix; Klein, Olivier; António, João H. C.; Babinska, Maria; Bangerter, Adrian; Bilewicz, Michal; Blanuša, Nebojša; Bovan, Kosta (17. januar 2022). «Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries». Nature Human Behaviour (på engelsk): 1–12. ISSN 2397-3374. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7. Besøkt 30. januar 2022. «We conclude that conspiracy mentality is associated with extreme left- and especially extreme right-wing beliefs, and that this non-linear relation may be strengthened by, but is not reducible to, deprivation of political control.» 

nb.no

urn.nb.no

nrk.no

omny.fm

rb.no

  • ««Lå det en sammensvergelse bak valget av Gardermoen?»». www.rb.no (på norsk). 29. august 2014. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Konspirasjonsteorier kan være svært underholdende lesestoff, men påstandene om en sammensvergelse fra lyssky Gardermoen-krefter er ikke av ny dato. Dette er slett ikke den første boka i rekka om Wiborgs skjebne. I 2000 kom journalist Ebbe Ording med «Historien om Gardermoen – et nasjonalt bedrageri», og også Ording mer enn antyder at Wiborg ble tatt av dage for å hindre en storpolitisk skandale.» 

sagepub.com

spp.sagepub.com

sciencedirect.com

  • Swami, Viren; Voracek, Martin; Stieger, Stefan; Tran, Ulrich S.; Furnham, Adrian (1. desember 2014). «Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories». Cognition. 3. 133: 572–585. ISSN 0010-0277. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Belief in conspiracy theories has been associated with a range of negative health, civic, and social outcomes, requiring reliable methods of reducing such belief. Thinking dispositions have been highlighted as one possible factor associated with belief in conspiracy theories, but actual relationships have only been infrequently studied. In Study 1, we examined associations between belief in conspiracy theories and a range of measures of thinking dispositions in a British sample (N = 990). Results indicated that a stronger belief in conspiracy theories was significantly associated with lower analytic thinking and open-mindedness and greater intuitive thinking. In Studies 2–4, we examined the causational role played by analytic thinking in relation to conspiracist ideation. In Study 2 (N = 112), we showed that a verbal fluency task that elicited analytic thinking reduced belief in conspiracy theories. In Study 3 (N = 189), we found that an alternative method of eliciting analytic thinking, which related to cognitive disfluency, was effective at reducing conspiracist ideation in a student sample. In Study 4, we replicated the results of Study 3 among a general population sample (N = 140) in relation to generic conspiracist ideation and belief in conspiracy theories about the July 7, 2005, bombings in London. Our results highlight the potential utility of supporting attempts to promote analytic thinking as a means of countering the widespread acceptance of conspiracy theories.» 
  • Hornsey, Matthew J.; Finlayson, Matthew; Chatwood, Gabrielle; Begeny, Christopher T. (1. mai 2020). «Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (på engelsk). 88: 103947. ISSN 0022-1031. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103947. Besøkt 17. desember 2021. 

skup.no

taylorfrancis.com

theguardian.com

universitypressscholarship.com

oxford.universitypressscholarship.com

voanews.com

web.archive.org

wiley.com

onlinelibrary.wiley.com

  • Sunstein, Cass R.; Vermeule, Adrian (2009). «Conspiracy Theories: Causes and Cures». Journal of Political Philosophy. 2 (på engelsk). 17: 202–227. ISSN 1467-9760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «“THE truth is out there”: conspiracy theories are all around us. In August 2004, a poll by Zogby International showed that 49 percent of New York City residents, with a margin of error of 3.5 percent, believed that officials of the U.S. government “knew in advance that attacks were planned on or around September 11, 2001, and that they consciously failed to act.” In a Scripps‐Howard Poll in 2006, some 36 percent of respondents assented to the claim that “federal officials either participated in the attacks on the World Trade Center or took no action to stop them.” Sixteen percent said that it was either very likely or somewhat likely that “the collapse of the twin towers in New York was aided by explosives secretly planted in the two buildings.”» 
  • Crenshaw, Martha (2000). «The Psychology of Terrorism: An Agenda for the 21st Century». Political Psychology. 2 (på engelsk). 21: 405–420. ISSN 0162-895X. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00195. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Oliver, J. Eric; Wood, Thomas J. (2014). «Conspiracy Theories and the Paranoid Style(s) of Mass Opinion». American Journal of Political Science. 4 (på engelsk). 58: 952–966. ISSN 1540-5907. doi:10.1111/ajps.12084. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Although conspiracy theories have long been a staple of American political culture, no research has systematically examined the nature of their support in the mass public. Using four nationally representative surveys, sampled between 2006 and 2011, we find that half of the American public consistently endorses at least one conspiracy theory and that many popular conspiracy theories are differentiated along ideological and anomic dimensions. In contrast with many theoretical speculations, we do not find conspiracism to be a product of greater authoritarianism, ignorance, or political conservatism. Rather, the likelihood of supporting conspiracy theories is strongly predicted by a willingness to believe in other unseen, intentional forces and an attraction to Manichean narratives. These findings both demonstrate the widespread allure of conspiracy theories as political explanations and offer new perspectives on the forces that shape mass opinion and American political culture. … Self‐described liberals score over one point lower, on average, on the ideological conspiracy scale than self‐described conservatives.» 
  • Jolley, Daniel; Douglas, Karen M. (februar 2014). «The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one's carbon footprint». British Journal of Psychology (på engelsk). 105 (1): 35–56. doi:10.1111/bjop.12018. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 

worldcat.org

  • Napolitano, M. Giulia; Reuter, Kevin (24. september 2021). «What is a Conspiracy Theory?». Erkenntnis (på engelsk). ISSN 1572-8420. doi:10.1007/s10670-021-00441-6. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Stojanov, Ana; Halberstadt, Jamin (1. juli 2019). «The Conspiracy Mentality Scale». Social Psychology. 4. 50: 215–232. ISSN 1864-9335. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000381. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Coady, David (27. april 2021). «Conspiracy theory as heresy». Educational Philosophy and Theory. 0. 0: 1–5. ISSN 0013-1857. doi:10.1080/00131857.2021.1917364. Besøkt 20. oktober 2022. 
  • Clarke, Steve (2002). «Conspiracy Theories and Conspiracy Theorizing». Philosophy of the Social Sciences. 2 (på engelsk). 32: 131–150. ISSN 0048-3931. doi:10.1177/004931032002001. Besøkt 6. november 2019. 
  • Sunstein, Cass R.; Vermeule, Adrian (2009). «Conspiracy Theories: Causes and Cures». Journal of Political Philosophy. 2 (på engelsk). 17: 202–227. ISSN 1467-9760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «“THE truth is out there”: conspiracy theories are all around us. In August 2004, a poll by Zogby International showed that 49 percent of New York City residents, with a margin of error of 3.5 percent, believed that officials of the U.S. government “knew in advance that attacks were planned on or around September 11, 2001, and that they consciously failed to act.” In a Scripps‐Howard Poll in 2006, some 36 percent of respondents assented to the claim that “federal officials either participated in the attacks on the World Trade Center or took no action to stop them.” Sixteen percent said that it was either very likely or somewhat likely that “the collapse of the twin towers in New York was aided by explosives secretly planted in the two buildings.”» 
  • Barkun, Michael (1. mars 1996). «Religion, militias and Oklahoma city: The mind of conspiratorialists». Terrorism and Political Violence. 1. 8: 50–64. ISSN 0954-6553. doi:10.1080/09546559608427332. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Crenshaw, Martha (2000). «The Psychology of Terrorism: An Agenda for the 21st Century». Political Psychology. 2 (på engelsk). 21: 405–420. ISSN 0162-895X. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00195. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Barkun, Michael (4. mai 2017). «President Trump and the “Fringe”». Terrorism and Political Violence. 3. 29: 437–443. ISSN 0954-6553. doi:10.1080/09546553.2017.1313649. Besøkt 9. april 2023. 
  • Swami, Viren; Voracek, Martin; Stieger, Stefan; Tran, Ulrich S.; Furnham, Adrian (1. desember 2014). «Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories». Cognition. 3. 133: 572–585. ISSN 0010-0277. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Belief in conspiracy theories has been associated with a range of negative health, civic, and social outcomes, requiring reliable methods of reducing such belief. Thinking dispositions have been highlighted as one possible factor associated with belief in conspiracy theories, but actual relationships have only been infrequently studied. In Study 1, we examined associations between belief in conspiracy theories and a range of measures of thinking dispositions in a British sample (N = 990). Results indicated that a stronger belief in conspiracy theories was significantly associated with lower analytic thinking and open-mindedness and greater intuitive thinking. In Studies 2–4, we examined the causational role played by analytic thinking in relation to conspiracist ideation. In Study 2 (N = 112), we showed that a verbal fluency task that elicited analytic thinking reduced belief in conspiracy theories. In Study 3 (N = 189), we found that an alternative method of eliciting analytic thinking, which related to cognitive disfluency, was effective at reducing conspiracist ideation in a student sample. In Study 4, we replicated the results of Study 3 among a general population sample (N = 140) in relation to generic conspiracist ideation and belief in conspiracy theories about the July 7, 2005, bombings in London. Our results highlight the potential utility of supporting attempts to promote analytic thinking as a means of countering the widespread acceptance of conspiracy theories.» 
  • Wood, Michael J.; Douglas, Karen M.; Sutton, Robbie M. (25. januar 2012). «Dead and Alive». Social Psychological and Personality Science. 6 (på engelsk). 3: 767–773. ISSN 1948-5506. doi:10.1177/1948550611434786. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: A self-sustaining worldview comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated in endorsement. In Study 1 (n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another but by broader beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.» 
  • Oliver, J. Eric; Wood, Thomas J. (2014). «Conspiracy Theories and the Paranoid Style(s) of Mass Opinion». American Journal of Political Science. 4 (på engelsk). 58: 952–966. ISSN 1540-5907. doi:10.1111/ajps.12084. Besøkt 5. november 2019. «Although conspiracy theories have long been a staple of American political culture, no research has systematically examined the nature of their support in the mass public. Using four nationally representative surveys, sampled between 2006 and 2011, we find that half of the American public consistently endorses at least one conspiracy theory and that many popular conspiracy theories are differentiated along ideological and anomic dimensions. In contrast with many theoretical speculations, we do not find conspiracism to be a product of greater authoritarianism, ignorance, or political conservatism. Rather, the likelihood of supporting conspiracy theories is strongly predicted by a willingness to believe in other unseen, intentional forces and an attraction to Manichean narratives. These findings both demonstrate the widespread allure of conspiracy theories as political explanations and offer new perspectives on the forces that shape mass opinion and American political culture. … Self‐described liberals score over one point lower, on average, on the ideological conspiracy scale than self‐described conservatives.» 
  • Imhoff, Roland; Zimmer, Felix; Klein, Olivier; António, João H. C.; Babinska, Maria; Bangerter, Adrian; Bilewicz, Michal; Blanuša, Nebojša; Bovan, Kosta (17. januar 2022). «Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries». Nature Human Behaviour (på engelsk): 1–12. ISSN 2397-3374. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7. Besøkt 30. januar 2022. «We conclude that conspiracy mentality is associated with extreme left- and especially extreme right-wing beliefs, and that this non-linear relation may be strengthened by, but is not reducible to, deprivation of political control.» 
  • Eberl, Jakob-Moritz; Huber, Robert A.; Greussing, Esther (31. mai 2021). «From populism to the “plandemic”: why populists believe in COVID-19 conspiracies». Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. sup1. 31: 272–284. ISSN 1745-7289. doi:10.1080/17457289.2021.1924730. Besøkt 17. desember 2021. «[T]he complex nature of the COVID-19 pandemic makes it an ideal playground for populists’ opposition to scientific and political elites. […] We demonstrate a negative correlation of populist attitudes with both trust in political and scientific institutions, which, in return, negatively relate to COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. This results in an overall positive relationship of populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs that is independent of political ideology.» 
  • Hornsey, Matthew J.; Finlayson, Matthew; Chatwood, Gabrielle; Begeny, Christopher T. (1. mai 2020). «Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (på engelsk). 88: 103947. ISSN 0022-1031. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103947. Besøkt 17. desember 2021.